Thompson sporadic group: Difference between revisions

From formulasearchengine
Jump to navigation Jump to search
en>Addbot
m Bot: Migrating 1 interwiki links, now provided by Wikidata on d:q3117700
en>R.e.b.
 
(One intermediate revision by one other user not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{More footnotes|date=January 2009}}
Myrtle Benny is how I'm called and I feel comfortable when people use the complete name. The favorite hobby for my kids and me is to play baseball and I'm attempting to make it a occupation. Years ago we moved to North Dakota. Hiring is her day occupation now but she's usually needed her personal company.<br><br>Visit my website [http://Bit.ly/1tS0F7g Bit.ly]
{{merge|Evoked potential|discuss=Evoked_potential#Evoked_Potential_Synonymous_with_Event-Related_Potential|date=October 2013}}
[[File:ComponentsofERP.svg|right|thumb|A waveform showing several ERP components, including the [[N100 (neuroscience)|N100]] and [[P300 (neuroscience)|P300]]. Note that the ERP is plotted with negative voltages upward, a common, but not universal, practice in ERP research]]
An '''event-related potential''' ('''ERP''') is the measured [[brain]] response that is the direct result of a specific [[sense|sensory]], [[cognition|cognitive]], or [[motor system|motor]] event.<ref name="Luck">{{Cite book | last = Luck | first = Steven J. | title = An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique | publisher = The MIT Press | year = 2005 | isbn =0-262-12277-4 }}</ref> More formally, it is any stereotyped [[electrophysiology|electrophysiological]] response to a stimulus. The study of the brain in this way provides a [[Invasiveness of surgical procedures|noninvasive]] means of evaluating brain functioning in patients with cognitive diseases.
 
ERPs are measured by means of [[electroencephalography]] (EEG).  The [[magnetoencephalography]] (MEG) equivalent of ERP is the ERF, or event-related field.<ref>{{cite book | editor=Colin M. Brown and Peter Hagoort | last=Brown | first=Colin M | coauthors=Peter Hagoort | chapter=The cognitive neuroscience of language | title=The Neurocognition of Language | year=1999 | publisher=[[Oxford University Press]] | location=New York | page=6}}</ref>
 
==History==
 
With the discovery of the [[electroencephalogram]] (EEG) in 1929, [[Hans Berger]] revealed that one could measure the electrical activity of the human brain by placing electrodes on the scalp and intensifying the signal. Changes in voltage can then be plotted over a period of time. He observed that the voltages could be influenced by external events that stimulated the senses. The EEG proved to be a useful source in recording brain activity over the ensuing decades. However, it tended to be very difficult to assess the highly specific neural process that are the focus of cognitive neuroscience because using pure EEG data made it difficult to isolate individual [[neurocognitive]] processes. Event-related potentials (ERPs) offered a more sophisticated method of extracting more specific sensory, cognitive, and motor events by using simple averaging techniques.
In 1935-1936 Pauline and [[Hallowell Davis]] recorded the first known ERPs on awake humans and their findings were published a few years later, in 1939. Due to [[World War II]] not much research was conducted in the 1940s, but research focusing on sensory issues picked back up again in the 1950s. In 1964, research by [[Grey Walter]] and colleagues began the modern era of ERP component discoveries when they reported the first cognitive ERP component, called the [[contingent negative variation]] (CNV).<ref>Walter, W.G; Cooper, R.; Aldridge, V.J.; McCallum, W.C.; Winter, A.L. (1964). "Contingent Negative Variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain". Nature 203 (4943): 380–384.</ref> Sutton, Braren, and Zubin (1965) made another advancement with the discovery of the P3 component.<ref>Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E.R. (1965). Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty. Science, 150, 1187-1188</ref> Over the next fifteen years, ERP component research became increasingly popular. The 1980s, with the introduction of inexpensive computers, opened up a new door for cognitive neuroscience research. Currently, ERP is one of the most widely used methods in [[cognitive neuroscience]] research to study the [[physiological]] correlates of [[sensory]], [[perceptual]] and [[cognitive]] activity associated with processing information.<ref>Handy, T. C. (2005). Event Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.</ref>
 
==Calculation==
 
ERPs can be [[Reliability (statistics)|reliably]] measured using [[electroencephalograph]]y (EEG), a procedure that measures [[electricity|electrical]] activity of the brain over time using [[electrode]]s placed on the [[scalp]]. The EEG reflects thousands of simultaneously [[Ongoing brain activity|ongoing brain processes]]. This means that the brain response to a single stimulus or event of interest is not usually visible in the EEG recording of a single trial. To see the brain's response to a stimulus, the experimenter must conduct many trials (100 or more: reference?) and average the results together, causing random brain activity to be averaged out and the relevant waveform to remain, called the ERP.<ref>{{cite book | url=http://l3d.cs.colorado.edu/~ctg/classes/lib/cogsci/Rugg-ColesChp1.pdf | chapter=Event-related brain potentials: an introduction | year=1996 | title=Electrophysiology of Mind | pages=1&ndash;27 | publisher=Oxford Scholarship Online Monographs | last=Coles | coauthors=[[Michael D. Rugg]] | first=Michael G.H.}}</ref>
 
The random ([[Neural_oscillation#Ongoing_activity|background]]) brain activity together with other bio-signals (e.g., [[Electrooculography|EOG]], [[Electromyography|EMG]], [[Electrocardiography|EKG]]) and electromagnetic interference (e.g., [[Noise (electronics)|line noise]], fluorescent lamps) constitute the noise contribution to the recorded ERP. This noise obscures the signal of interest, which is the sequence of underlying ERPs under study.
From an engineering point of view it is possible to define the signal-to-noise ratio ([[Signal-to-noise ratio|SNR]]) of the recorded ERPs. The reason that averaging increases the SNR of the recorded ERPs (making them discernible and allowing for their interpretation) has a simple mathematical explanation provided that some simplifying assumptions are made. These assumptions are:
# The signal of interest is made of a sequence of event-locked ERPs with invariable latency and shape
# The noise can be approximated by a zero-mean [[Gaussian process|Gaussian random process]] of variance <math>\sigma^2</math> which is uncorrelated between trials and not time-locked to the event (this assumption can be easily violated, for example in the case of a subject doing little tongue movements while mentally counting the targets in an [[oddball paradigm]]).
Having defined <math>k</math>, the trial number, and <math>t</math>, the time elapsed after the <math>k</math><sup>th</sup> event, each recorded trial can be written as <math>x(t,k)=s(t)+n(t,k)</math> where <math>s(t)</math> is the signal and <math>n(t,k)</math> is the noise (Note that, under the assumptions above, the signal does not depend on the specific trial while the noise does).
 
The average of <math>N</math> trials is
:<math>\bar x(t) = \frac{1}{N} \sum_{k=1}^N x(t,k) = s(t) + \frac{1}{N} \sum_{k=1}^N n(t,k)</math> .
 
The [[expected value]] of <math>\bar x(t)</math> is (as hoped) the signal itself, <math>\operatorname{E}[\bar x(t)] = s(t)</math>.
 
Its [[variance]] is
:<math>\operatorname{Var}[\bar x(t)] = \operatorname{E}\left[\left(\bar x(t) - \operatorname{E}[\bar x(t)]\right)^2\right] = \frac{1}{N^2} \operatorname{E}\left[\left(\sum_{k=1}^N n(t,k)\right)^2\right] = \frac{1}{N^2} \sum_{k=1}^N \operatorname{E}\left[n(t,k)^2\right] = \frac{\sigma^2}{N}</math>.
For this reason the noise amplitude of the average of <math>N</math> trials is <math>1/{\sqrt{N}}</math> times that of a single trial.
 
Wide amplitude noise (such as eye blinks or movement [[Artifact (error)|artifacts]]) are often several orders of magnitude larger than the underlying ERPs. Therefore, trials containing such artifacts should be removed before averaging. Artifact rejection can be performed manually by visual inspection or using an automated procedure based on predefined fixed thresholds (limiting the maximum EEG amplitude or slope) or on time-varying thresholds derived from the statistics of the set of trials.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/33207 |title=ERP_REJECT, rejection of outlier trials from ERP studies |author= |publisher=Matlab File Exchange |accessdate=December 30, 2011}}</ref>
 
==ERP Components Nomenclature==
 
ERP waveforms consist of a series of positive and negative voltage deflections, which are related to a set of underlying '''components'''.<ref>{{cite book|title=The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components|year=2012|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=9780195374148|pages=664|url=http://www.oup.com/us/catalog/general/subject/Psychology/CognitivePsychology/?view=usa&ci=9780195374148|editor=Luck, S.J., and Kappenman, E.S.}}</ref> Though some ERP components are referred to with acronyms (e.g., [[contingent negative variation]] - CNV, [[error-related negativity]] - ERN, [[early left anterior negativity]] - ELAN, [[closure positive shift]] - CPS), most components are referred to by a letter (N/P) indicating polarity (negative/positive), followed by a number indicating either the latency in milliseconds or the component's [[Ordinal number|ordinal]] position in the waveform. For instance, a negative-going peak that is the first substantial peak in the waveform and often occurs about 100 milliseconds after a stimulus is presented is often called the [[N100 (neuroscience)|N100]] (indicating its latency is 100 ms after the stimulus and that it is negative) or N1 (indicating that it is the first peak and is negative); it is often followed by a positive peak, usually called the [[P200]] or P2. The stated latencies for ERP components are often quite variable. For example, the [[P300 (neuroscience)|P300]] component may exhibit a peak anywhere between 250ms - 700ms.<ref>For discussion of ERP component naming conventions see Luck, Steven (2005), ''An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique'', MIT Press, pp. 10-11.</ref>
While [[evoked potential]]s reflect the processing of the physical stimulus, event-related potentials are caused by the "higher" processes, that might involve [[memory]], [[Expectation (epistemic)|expectation]], [[attention]], or changes in the mental state, among others.{{fact|date=October 2013}}
 
==Relative Advantages & Disadvantages==
 
===ERP vs Behavioral Measures===
Compared with behavioral procedures, ERPs provide a continuous measure of processing between a stimulus and a response, making it possible to determine which stage(s) are being affected by a specific experimental manipulation. Another advantage over behavioral measures is that they can provide a measure of processing of stimuli even when there is no behavioral change. However, because of the significantly small size of an ERP, it usually takes a large sample size to accurately measure it correctly.<ref>Luck, Steven (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. MIT Press, pp. 21-23.</ref>
 
===ERP vs Other Physiological Measures===
 
====Invasiveness====
Unlike microelectrodes which require an electrode to be inserted into the brain and [[Positron emission tomography|PET]] scans that expose humans to radiation, ERPs use EEG, a non-invasive procedure.
 
====Spatial and Temporal Resolution====
 
ERPs provide excellent [[temporal resolution]] of 1 ms or better. The [[spatial resolution]] of an ERP, however, is currently undefined making it impossible to definitively localize ERPs. This provides the ERP with a major disadvantage over [[hemodynamic]] measures ([[fMRI]] and [[Positron emission tomography|PET]]) which have a spatial resolution in the millimeter range. The fact that ERPs cannot easily be localized makes it extremely difficult to isolate a single ERP component from the overall ERP component.<ref>{{cite book|last=Luck|first=Steven|title=An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique|year=2005|publisher=MIT Press|location=Massachusetts Institute of Technology|isbn=0-262-62196-7|pages=25–26}}</ref>
 
===Cost===
 
ERP research is much cheaper to do than other imaging techniques such as [[fMRI]] and [[Positron emission tomography|PET]]. This is because purchasing and maintaining an EEG system is less expensive than the other systems.
 
==Clinical ERP==
[[Physician]]s and [[neurology|neurologists]] will sometimes use a flashing [[Visual perception|visual]] checkerboard stimulus to test for any damage or trauma in the visual system. In a healthy person, this stimulus will elicit a strong response over the primary [[visual cortex]] located in the [[occipital lobe]], in the back of the brain.
 
ERP component abnormalities in clinical research have been shown in neurological conditions such as:
*dementia<ref>Boutros, N., et al. (1995). Evoked potentials in subjects at risk for Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Res, 57, (1), 57-63.</ref>
*Parkinson's disease<ref>Prabhakar, S., Syal, P. and Srivastava, T. (2000). P300 in newly diagnosed nondementing
Parkinson's disease: effect of dopaminergic drugs. Neurol India, 48, (3), 239-
242.</ref>
*multiple sclerosis<ref>Boose, M. A. and Cranford, J. L. (1996). Auditory event-related potentials in multiple sclerosis. Am J Otol, 17, (1), 165-170.</ref>
*head injuries<ref>Duncan, C. C., Kosmidis, M. H. and Mirsky, A. F. (2003). Event-related potential assessment of information processing after closed head injury. Psychophysiology, 40, (1), 45-59.</ref>
*stroke<ref>D'Arcy, R. C., et al. (2003). Electrophysiological assessment of language function following stroke. Clin Neurophysiol, 114, (4), 662-672.</ref>
*Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder<ref>Hanna, G.L., Carrasco, M., Harbin, S.M., Nienhuis, J.K., LaRosa, C.E., Chen, P., Fitzgerald, K.D., Gehring, W.J. (2012).  Error-Related Negativity and Tic History in Pediatric Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder.  Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, (9), 902-910.</ref>
 
==Research ERP==
ERPs are used extensively in [[neuroscience]], [[cognitive psychology]], [[cognitive science]], and [[Psychophysiology|psycho-physiological]] research. [[Experimental psychology|Experimental psychologists]] and [[neuroscience|neuroscientists]] have discovered many different stimuli that elicit reliable ERPs from participants. The timing of these responses is thought to provide a measure of the timing of the brain's communication or timing of information processing. For example, in the checkerboard paradigm described above, healthy participants' first response of the visual cortex is around 50-70 ms. This would seem to indicate that this is the amount of time it takes for the [[Transduction (physiology)|transduced]] visual stimulus to reach the [[telencephalon|cortex]] after [[light]] first enters the [[eye]]. Alternatively, the [[P300 (neuroscience)|P300]] response occurs at around 300ms in the [[oddball paradigm]], for example, regardless of the type of stimulus presented: [[Visual system|visual]], [[tactition|tactile]], [[sound|auditory]], [[olfaction|olfactory]], [[gustatory]], etc. Because of this general invariance with regard to stimulus type, the P300 component is understood to reflect a higher cognitive response to unexpected and/or cognitively [[Salience (neuroscience)|salient]] stimuli.
 
Due to the consistency of the P300 response to novel stimuli, a [[brain-computer interface]] can be constructed which relies on it.  By arranging many signals in a grid, randomly flashing the rows of the grid as in the previous paradigm, and observing the P300 responses of a subject staring at the grid, the subject may communicate which stimulus he is looking at, and thus slowly "type" words.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Farwell|first=L.A.|coauthors=Donchin E.|title=Talking off the top of your head: toward a mental prosthesis utilizing event-related brain potentials|journal=Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol.|year=1988|volume=70|issue=6|pages=510–23|pmid=2461285|url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2461285|accessdate=5 December 2011|doi=10.1016/0013-4694(88)90149-6}}</ref>
 
Other ERPs used frequently in research, especially [[neurolinguistics|neurolinguistics research]], include the [[ELAN (neurolinguistics)|ELAN]], the [[N400 (neuroscience)|N400]], and the [[P600 (neuroscience)|P600/SPS]].
 
==ERP Software and Training Resources==
* [http://sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab/ EEGLAB Toolbox]- A freely available, open source, Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing EEG data
* [http://erpinfo.org/erplab ERPLAB Toolbox]- A freely available, open source, Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing ERP data
* [http://erpinfo.org/the-erp-bootcamp/bootcamp The ERP Boot Camp]- A series of training workshops for ERP researchers
 
==See also==
* [[Erich Schröger]]
* [[Evoked potential]]
* [[Induced activity]]
* [[Somatosensory evoked potential]]
* [[C1 & P1 (Neuroscience)|C1 and P1]]
* [[Mismatch negativity]]
* Negativity: [[N100 (neuroscience)|N100]] - [[Visual N1]] - [[N170]] - [[N200 (neuroscience)|N200]] - [[N2pc]] - [[N400 (neuroscience)|N400]]
* Positivity: [[P200]] -  [[P300 (neuroscience)|P300]] - [[P3a]] - [[P3b]] - [[Late Positive Component]] - [[P600 (neuroscience)|P600]]
* [[Difference due to Memory]]
* [[Contingent negative variation]]
* [[Error-related negativity]]
* [[Bereitschaftspotential]]
* [[Lateralized readiness potential]]
* [[Early left anterior negativity]]
 
==Further reading==
 
*  Steven J. Luck: ''An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique''. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press, 2005. ISBN 0-262-62196-7
*  Todd C. Handy: ''Event-Related Potentials : A Methods Handbook''. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press (B&T), 2004. ISBN 0-262-08333-7
* Luck, S.J., and Kappenman, E.S., ed. (2012). [http://www.oup.com/us/catalog/general/subject/Psychology/CognitivePsychology/?view=usa&ci=9780195374148 The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components]. Oxford University Press. pp.&nbsp;664. ISBN 9780195374148.
* Monica Fabiani, Gabriele Gratton, and Kara D. Federmeier: "Event-Related Brain Potentials: Methods, Theory, and Applications". In ''Handbook of Psychophysiology'', ed. by John T. Cacioppo, Louis G. Tassinary, and Gary G. Berntson. 3rd. ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2007. ISBN 978-0-521-84471-0. pp.&nbsp;85–119
* John Polich and Jody Corey-Bloom, ''Alzheimer's Disease and P300: Review and evaluation of Task and Modality''. '''Current Alzheimer Research''', 2005, 2, 515-525
* Zani A. & Proverbio A.M. (2003) ''Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind and Brain''. Academic Press/Elsvier.
* Kropotov J. (2009)" Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy" Academic Press/Elsvier.
 
==References==
{{reflist}}
 
{{EEG}}
 
{{DEFAULTSORT:Event-Related Potential}}
[[Category:Evoked potentials]]
[[Category:Electroencephalography]]
[[Category:Neurophysiology]]
 
[[de:Ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale]]
[[fr:Event-Related Potential]]
[[ko:사건 관련 전위]]
[[it:Potenziale evento-correlato]]
[[ja:事象関連電位]]
[[pl:Potencjały wywołane]]
[[pt:Potenciais de eventos relacionados]]
[[fi:ERP (fysiologia)]]
[[zh:事件相关电位]]

Latest revision as of 06:03, 11 October 2014

Myrtle Benny is how I'm called and I feel comfortable when people use the complete name. The favorite hobby for my kids and me is to play baseball and I'm attempting to make it a occupation. Years ago we moved to North Dakota. Hiring is her day occupation now but she's usually needed her personal company.

Visit my website Bit.ly